But during that second, a number of events will have transpired in the mind of the Reader. If we knew the train of events, we would know about the different reading processes.
Many models have been put forth to describe the essential elements involved in reading, but none has proven to be as robust and useful as what Philip Gough described as the "Simple View" of reading.
In this model, there are two general elements that are equally important to reading comprehension -- they are decoding skills and language comprehension skills.
Put differently, a person's ability to read and understand text can be predicted if you know that person's ability to decode words and that person's ability to understand spoken language. If a child is rapid and fluent at decoding text unfamiliar words or pseudowords for preferenceAND that child has no difficulty understanding spoken language implicit understanding of more formal language for preferencethen you can safely predict that child will not have difficulty with independent reading comprehension.
Research has shown repeatedly that, in broad strokes, when there are deficits in reading comprehension, there are always deficits in either language comprehension or decoding skills, or both -- in fact, most typically both.
There are some children although it is rare whose reading comprehension suffers because, while they are very good at understanding spoken language, they are poor at decoding text -- these children are called true dyslexics.
There are other children although very rare whose reading comprehension suffers because, while they are good at decoding text, they have difficulties understanding spoken language -- these children are called hyperlexics.
Most commonly, when a child has difficulty with reading comprehension, they are having difficulty with both decoding and language comprehension -- these children are described as having garden variety reading difficulties.
Somebody with perfect ability in one area would get a 1, and somebody who completely lacks ability in that area gets a 0 with most people falling somewhere in between.
It makes sense to think of the relationship as multiplicative, then, because if you a child is lacking in either decoding skills D or language comprehension skills Cthen reading skills R comparably suffer. Consider a child who gets a perfect score on a language comprehension assessment a score of 1but who performs poorly on a decoding assessment a score of 0.
This model would predict that child would perform poorly a score of 0 on a reading comprehension assessment. Children who are poor at decoding isolated words are also poor at reading and understanding text, regardless of how good they are at understanding language.
Similarly, children who are poor at understanding language are poor at reading and understanding text, regardless of how good they are at decoding words. Researchers like Ron Carver have confirmed these predictions many times over. And the other side of the coin has also been confirmed -- children who are good at decoding isolated words fast, fluent, and accurate and who are good at understanding spoken language are, on the whole, quite good at reading and understanding connected text.
The predictions made by the Simple View of Reading have been confirmed many times over. First, we as educators must learn what it takes to help children to be both good decoders and good language comprehenders.
And second, we should structure our assessment around this model. There is a framework of reading acquisition based on the Simple View that addresses the first implication -- this framework outlines the essential cognitive elements that research has shown to be important in the development of good decoding skills and good language comprehension skills.
The complete PDF version of the framework document can be download here for free, or an on-line interactive version of the framework is available at the Southwest Educational Development Laboratory website. Structuring assessment information around this framework is trivially easy -- there is an on-line reading assessment database that allows you to search for published assessments that test specific knowledge domains that are outlined in the framework.
Further, there is an overview document describing how each of these assessments is typically assessed. There are several ways to test a child's knowledge of, for example, phoneme awareness, and this document describes the different assessment approaches that can be used to get information about a child's growth in each of these knowledge domains.
Beginning of the year Simple Informal Student Reading Survey for grades By September of every school year, teachers should be able to answer this question -- how well do each of your students read? More specifically, how many students in your class this year are having difficulties with reading?
Do their difficulties stem from problems with decoding fluency or language comprehension? This is the first question that I try to answer when I encounter a student struggling with reading -- is it a decoding problem or a language comprehension problem?
Determining this is the first step in diagnosis, and it is the first step in planning intervention and effective instruction. Language comprehension problems are easy enough to identify -- if a student has trouble reading a passage of text, just read the passage of text out loud to the student.
If the student can understand the passage when she listens to you read it aloud, then her language comprehension skills are not preventing her from understanding the text.
She understands the material -- she just can't read it independently. It must be a decoding problem. Decoding is a little thornier, but not much. Most students with decoding problems are able to correctly identify words and "attack" unfamiliar words -- they just do it very, very slowly.
Those students have problems with decoding fluency. That's good news because fluency instruction is quite easy. Some students, however, are not able to accurately identify words -- especially unfamiliar words they have not seen before.Dec 07, · Gough () Information Processing Theory This article by Philip Gough () is a text-based analysis of reading, in which he cites research evidence of his own and others in putting forth a scientific analysis of how we read and what is going on hypothetically inside our minds to create meaning from the orthographic symbols of print.
Polygamy (from Late Greek πολυγαμία, polygamía, "state of marriage to many spouses") is the practice of marrying multiple spouses. When a man is married to more than one wife at a time, sociologists call this benjaminpohle.com a woman is married to more than one husband at a time, it is called benjaminpohle.com a marriage includes multiple husbands and .
SEDL merged with the American Institutes for Research (AIR) on January 1, This archived website contains the work of SEDL legacy projects and .
Committed to publishing great books, connecting readers and authors globally, and spreading the love of reading. Wanted: Digital copies of Group photographs, Scrapbooks, Autograph books, photo albums, newspaper clippings, letters, postcards and ephemera relating to benjaminpohle.com would like to obtain digital copies of any documents or photographs relating to WW2 you may have at home.
If you have any unwanted photographs, documents or items from the First or Second . – One second after his initial fixation, only the first word will have been uttered.
But during that second, a number of events will have transpired in the mind of the Reader. If we knew the train of events, we would know about the different reading processes.